Verbal Mnemonics

Techniques to Improve Memory: Verbal Mnemonics

Verbal Mnemonics are memory techniques using verbal aids to enable us remember things more easily. Research has shown that asking people to make up a story that links together a list of words makes later recall of those words much easier.

Although verbal mnemonics have not met with the same popularity as the visual imagery Opens in new window techniques, several verbal or non-visual strategies have been devised to improve memory.

The most widely used verbal mnemonic is a method for remembering how many days there are in each month of the year. For example, most Brits and Americans use a rhyme Opens in new window to do this (i.e., “Thirty days has September …”). In other parts of the world, many people use their knuckles, with the knuckles themselves representing the long months and the dips in between representing the short months. Still other countries use suffixes and prefixes to remember the long and short months. Every country using our calendar system has a mnemonic for remembering months of different lengths.

Types Verbal Mnemonics

The first-letter mnemonic Opens in new window is a method in which the initial letters of words in a sentence are used to recall information in a particular order. For example, many people in the United Kingdom use the mnemonic EGBDF when learning the notes on the lines of a musical staff. The letters are incorporated into a sentence such as “Every good boy deserves fruit.” After learning the sentence, one uses the initial letters for notes, so the note on the first line is E, the note on the second line is G, etc. An amiable author once said “My granddaughter recently taught me a first letter mnemonic for remembering the order of the planets from the sun: ‘My very elderly mother just sat upon a new pin’ (Mercury, Venus, Earth, Mars, Jupiter, Saturn, Uranus, Neptune, Pluto).” A variation on this theme is to use a whole word to remember the information so that the notes in the spaces of the musical staff spell out the word face. Harris (1984) suggested that first-letter mnemonics are useful only when the to-be-remembered material is well known but difficult to recall in the correct order. They can however, be used to learn new material (Wilson, 1987). Another variation can be used to remember a list of digits such as a credit card number: One can make up a sentence in which each word consists of a number of letters corresponding to the numbers to be remembered. For example, the number 6,734 can be “Mother (six digits) courage (seven) was (three) here (four).” Wilson and Moffat (1984) argued that first-letter mnemonics work for two reasons: (1) Because the information is being chunked and chunking has long been known to increase recall (Miller, 1956) and (2) because it reduces the number of competing responses. Elaboration or making words into a story is another method used to improve recall for people with and without brain lesions (Crovitz, 1979; Gianutsos & Gianutsos, 1987; Wilson, 1987). Crovitz used the “airplane list,” in which 10 words are embedded in a story with each word linked to the next. Wilson (1987) compared the story method with three other methods. The list of words to be remembered was umpire, nose, iceberg, vase, elephant, refugee, skylark, imp, tree, and yak (the first letters of these make the word university, as the first-letter mnemonic system was one of the methods studied. For the story method, the words were made into a story based on the procedure used by Crovitz (1979);

Mnemonics are used to help remember vocabulary, names, rules, lists, and other kinds of factual knowledge. Table underneath provides some variety of mnemonic devices according to common uses or characteristics; alternative terminology is shown in parentheses.

Basic devices used for storing and recalling list of words, or objects:
MnemonicDescriptionExample
1. Link method Opens in new window (interactive imagery)Visually linking items in a list into a series of overlapping images in a chain (may be used as an alternative to the peg-word mnemonic Opens in new window)A student visualizes homework stuck in a notebook, which is bound to his/her textbook, pencil, and pen with a rubber band to remember to take the (italicized) items to class
2. Method of Loci Opens in new windowAssociating a list of items with a sequence of fixed physical locations in familiar environments, such as the chair, sofa, lamp, end table, and footstool, in a living roomStudent wanting to remember the first five elements of the periodic table visualizes hydrogen at the chair, helium at the sofa, lithium at the lamp, etc
3. First letter recoding (acronym encoding)Creating a word out of the first letters of the items to be remembered A student creates the word Wajmma to remember the first six presidents in order: Washington, Adams, Jefferson, Madison, Monroe, and Adams
4. Peg-word Opens in new window (hook) strategyMemorizing a series of “pegs”—such as “one is bun” and “two is shoe”—on which to-be-remembered information is hungA person wanting to get pickles and carrots at the grocery store visualizes a pickle in a bun and a carrot stuck in a shoe
5. Key-word method Opens in new windowAssigning imagery and rhyming words to remember unfamiliar words A learner remembers that trigo, which rhymes with tree, is the Spanish word for wheat by visualizing a sheaf of wheat sticking out of a tree
Basic devices used for storing and recalling list of words, or objects:
MnemonicDescriptionExample
6. Syntactic encoding (natural language mediator)Involves associating items in a list with a preposition or a conjunction, or linking items in a phrase, clause, sentence or storyAs example, the list: dog, bone, hill becomes, the dog hid the bone on the hill
7. Conceptual categorizing Opens in new window (taxonomic grouping, or semantic categorizing)Recalling a large quantity of information into a simplified form by reorganizing the items into a few conceptual categoriesFor example, grouping food items in a list into categories, such as vegetables, meats, etc
8. Semantic encodingAssociating two or more words according to a common meaninge.g, berm and earplug = sound barriers
Substituting a single abstract word for a concrete word having the same meaninge.g., origin = egg
9. Phonetic encodingAssociating words that have similar speech sounds, or substituting abstract words for concrete words having similar speech soundse.g., Irrawaddy = ear wad
10. Bridging strategyAssociating two or more words with an intermediate word with which the other words are usually associatede.g., associating the words soup and letter with the word alphabet
Mnemonic devices used for storing and retrieving numbers
MnemonicDescriptionExample
1. Digit-consonant encoding (analytic substitution)Substituting letters for numbers to form words e.g., when 1=1; 2=n and 3=m
2. ChunkingAnalyzing an unbroken sequence of numbers into smaller units to assist retention e.g., 436-7529.

Through the use of mnemonic techniques some spectacular results in recall performance have been obtained (Bellezza, 1981). Bower and Clark (1969), for example, found 93 percent recall in a mnemonic group, compared to 13 percent in a control group.

Ericsson, Chase and Faloon (1980), worked with a college student of average intelligence and memory ability whose memory span after 230 hours of practice increased from 7 to 79 digits. His performance on memory tests of digits equaled that of memory experts with life-long training. The authors concluded that, with an appropriate mnemonic system, retrieval method and practice there is seemingly no limit to memory skills.

  1. M. W. Eysenck (1994) Perspectives on psychology (Hove, UK: Psychology Press).
  2. David Baine, Memory and Instruction (Chunking and Categorizing P. 42-43).
  3. J.C. Berryman, D.J. Hargreaves, C.R. Hollin, and K. Howells (1978) Psychology and you (Leicester, UK: BPS Books).
  4. C. Tavris and C. Wade (1997) Psychology in perspective (New York: Longman).
  5. Rod Plotnik, Haig Kouyoumdjian Introduction to Psychology (Chunking P. 241)
Image